Drawing from...
Bandura, A.: Mechanisms of Moral Disengagement
in W. Reich
(Ed.), Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies, Theologies, States of
Mind (pp. 161-191), Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 1990.
(Bandura's article can be seen in its
entirety at
https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/2c93/c0e54861a54874811c1207fcc068e845f302.pdf. Albert Bandura is one of the biggest names in behaviorism and behavior modification during the mid-to-late 20th century.)
Given...
2) Eric Hoffer's observations in his classic book, The True
Believer,
3) Robert Altemeyer's research on the mechanisms of
authoritarianism at the U. of Manitoba,
4) Lawrence Kohlberg's six stages of moral reasoning,
5) Stanley Milgram's famed work on submission to authority, and
6) Otto Kernberg's notions of personality and organization thereof,
6) Otto Kernberg's notions of personality and organization thereof,
Bandura's submission to Reich's Origins of Terrorism neatly
ties together the dynamics of rationalized abuse of the "Gluttons for
Punishment" and "Willful Slaves" at levels seven and eight of
the pyramid by the "Willful Slaves" and "Cynics" at levels
eight and nine via cultic thought reform and careful manipulation of the
reward, reinforcement and punishment schemes of Pavlovian "classical" and
Watsonian-Skinnerian-Banduran "operant" conditioning and behavior modification by the "Cynics"
and "Sociopaths" at levels nine and ten.
Quoting Bandura:
"A number of social factors affect the ease with which
responsibility for one's actions can be surrendered to others. High
justification and social consensus [or "social proof"] about the
morality of an enterprise aid in the relinquishment of personal control. The
legitimacy of the authorizers [repeatedly conditioned, socialized and normalized at the lower levels on the cultic pyramid... and well established once cult
members have reached level seven] is another important determinant. The higher
the authorities, the more legitimacy, respect and coercive power they command,
and the more amenable are people [on the levels below] to defer to them."
"Obedient functionaries... tend to be conscientious and
self-directed in the performance of their duties. It requires a strong sense of
responsibility to be a good functionary. In situations involving obedience to
authority, people carry out orders partly to honor the obligations they have
undertaken. It is, therefore important to distinguish between two levels of
responsibility, duty to one's superiors and accountability for the effects of
one's actions. Self-sanctions operate most efficiently in the service of
authority when followers assume personal responsibility for being dutiful
executors while relinquishing personal responsibility for the harm caused by
their behavior. Followers who disowned responsibility without being bound by a
sense of duty would be quite unreliable."
"The deterrent power of [common culturally socialized]
self-sanctions is weakened when responsibility for culpable behavior is
diffused, thereby obscuring the link between conduct and its consequences. This
is achieved in several ways. Responsibility can be diffused by the division of
labor. Most enterprises require the services of many people, each performing
fragmentary jobs that seem harmless in themselves. The fractional contribution
is easily isolated from the eventual function, especially when participants
exercise little personal judgment in carrying out a subfunction that is related
by remote, complex links to the end result. After activities become routinized
into programmed subfunctions, attention shifts from the import of what one is
doing to the details of one's fractional job (Kelman, 1973).
"Group decision-making is another common bureaucratic
practice that enables otherwise considerate people to behave inhumanely,
because no single individual feels responsible for policies arrived at
collectively. Where everyone is responsible no one is really responsible.
Social organizations [cults included] go to great lengths to devise sophisticated mechanisms for
obscuring responsibility for decisions that will affect others adversely.
Collective action is still another diffusion expedient for weakening
self-restraints. Any harm done by a group can always be ascribed, in large
part, to the behavior of other members. People, therefore, act more harshly
when responsibility is obfuscated by a collective instrumentality than when
they hold themselves personally accountable for what they do (Bandura,
Underwood, & Fromson, 1975; Diener, 1977; Zimbardo, 1969)."
"Most organizations involve hierarchical chains of
command in which superiors formulate plans and intermediaries transmit them to
executors, who then carry them out. The further removed individuals are from
the end results, the weaker is the restraining power of the foreseeable
destructive effects. Kilham and Mann (1974) set forth the view that the
disengagement of personal control is easiest for the intermediaries in a
hierarchical [or pyramidic] system -- they neither bear responsibility for
major decisions nor are they a party to their execution. In performing the
transmitter role they model dutiful behavior and further legitimize their superiors
and their social policies and practices."
Thus, the the "Willful Slaves" and "Cynics" at levels eight and nine who have been de-conditioned to societally "normal" moral standards in the service of "getting the job done" (e.g.: "saving the world") for the "Cynics" and "Sociopaths" at levels nine and ten can order the "Gluttons for Punishment" and "Willful Slaves" at levels seven and eight of the pyramid to abuse the "Lab Rats" and "Gluttons for Punishment" at levels six and seven with both a) rationalization of the order-giving and b) reduced sense of responsibility for their actions. For the agendas of "Cynics" and "Sociopaths" at levels nine and ten, it's hard to think it could work any better.
In addition, a childhood path of having been conditioned, socialized and normalized to dichotomous, "either-or," "all-or-nothing," "entirely-right-or-entirely-wrong," and "all-good-or-all-evil" thinking (as is so often seen in morally perfectionistic, fundamentalist religiosity) is a fine set-up for the psychological "splitting" of morality and ethics seen in upper level cult members and leaders. Splitting is the bedrock of what Otto Kernberg called "borderline personality organization." And is is that, it seems to me, that one sees in the "Willful Slaves" and "Cynics," though the "Sociopaths" at level ten seem fully "integrated" (to use Kernberg's term) to fully rationalized, malignant narcissism.
In addition, a childhood path of having been conditioned, socialized and normalized to dichotomous, "either-or," "all-or-nothing," "entirely-right-or-entirely-wrong," and "all-good-or-all-evil" thinking (as is so often seen in morally perfectionistic, fundamentalist religiosity) is a fine set-up for the psychological "splitting" of morality and ethics seen in upper level cult members and leaders. Splitting is the bedrock of what Otto Kernberg called "borderline personality organization." And is is that, it seems to me, that one sees in the "Willful Slaves" and "Cynics," though the "Sociopaths" at level ten seem fully "integrated" (to use Kernberg's term) to fully rationalized, malignant narcissism.
Bandura's References
Bandura, A,; Underwood, B.; Fromson, M.: Disinhibition of
Aggression Through Diffusion of Responsibility and Dehumanization of Victims,
in Journal of Research in Personality, Vol. 9, 1975.
Diener, E.: Deindividuation: Causes and Consequences, in
Social Behavior and Personality, Vol. 5, 1977.
Kelman, H.: Violence Without Moral Restraint: Reflections on
the Dehumanization of Victims and Victimizers, in Journal of Social Issues, Vol.
29, 1973.
Kilham, W.; Mann, L.: Level of Destructive Obediences as a
Function of Transmittrer and Executant Roles in the Milgram Obedience Paradigm,
in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 29, 1974.
Zimbardo, P.; 1969, quoted in Zimbardo, P.: The Lucifer
Effect: Understanding How Good People Turn Evil, New York: Random House, 2007
Commentator's References
Altemeyer, R.: The Authoritarian Specter, Boston: Harvard
University Press, 1996.
Altemeyer, R.: The Authoritarians, Charleston, SC: Lulu,
2006.
Bandura, A.: Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control, San
Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1997.
Hoffer, E.: The True Believer: Thoughts on the Nature of
Mass Movements, New York: Harper and Row, 1951, 1966.
Kohlberg, L.: The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages, San Francisco: Harper & Row, 1984.
Milgram, S.: Obedience to Authority, London: Pinter &
Martin, 1974.
No comments:
Post a Comment