This is the second in a series of articles on the
psychiatric casualties of thought reform programs. The first, "Psychiatric
Casualties of Thought Reform Programs," was published here almost two
years ago and remains available for viewing. It was developed from Margaret
Thaler Singer's and Richard Ofshe's paper, Thought Reform
Programs and the Production of Psychiatric Casualties, originally
published in Psychiatric Annals in April 1990. The following article
was developed from an excerpt from Dr. Philip Cushman's "The
Politics of Transformation: Recruitment - Indoctrination Processes in a Mass
Marathon Psychology Organization," published by St. Martin's Press of
New York City in 1993, and retrieved from Rick Allen Ross's Cult Education
Forum at this
address. The original text by Dr. Cushman is in black; my comments are in
dark red.
Mass marathon training is usually based on the belief that it
is a universal truth that all human beings will have problems in life until
they develop deep cathartic psychological insight, experience completely their
every feeling, and live only in the present moment (see Brewer, 1975; Bry,
1976; Rhinehart, 1976). According to this ideology all defenses are bad and
must be destroyed. They shape their group exercises in order to uncover and
intensify the participants' underlying conflicts and deficits. Everyone must be
exposed to these exercises; there are no exceptions. When all defenses are
destroyed, they claim there is literally no limit to what each individual can
accomplish.
The phrase "great ideas badly delivered" hit me right between the eyes many years ago. So many of the concepts described and examined in the MM/LGATs that came from humanism and the human potential movement of the mid-2oth century are now part and parcel of highly effect, third-wave, mindfulness-based cognitive psychotherapies and fourth-wave, mind-body psychotherapies (like DBT, ACT and MBCT among the former, as well as MBBT, SEPt and SP4T among the latter). But for many who still associate those techniques with the suffering of their (usually later) cult experiences, such therapies are too often re-triggering and intolerable.
The mass marathon trainings (aka "large group awareness trainings" or LGATs) discussed in Cushman's work are pretty clearly of the sort popular in the 1970s and '80s, including the following (with all of which I am personally familiar):
Werner Erhard's est Training (later known as The Forum, and the Landmark Forum)
John Hanley Sr's Lifespring (which may be the particular example used in the material in the link at the end of this article though there is also a very strong -- but not 100% -- resemblance to what I experienced in several est trainings in the 1970s)
Tom (and later, Jane) Wilhite's PSI
Stewart Emery's Actualizations
Alexander Everett's Mind Dynamics
Justin Sterling's Sterling Institute of Relationship
(This is where I began to run into multiple, cross-platform, reformatting problems. Blogger is a very intolerant, early "cloud" platform. I will do my best here. Sigh.)
Yet there is research that contradicts this universal claim.
Applebaum (1976) reported on the results of the Psychotherapy Research Project
of the Henninger Foundation, which attempted to better understand the effects
of psychological insight in the treatment of patients who had ego-function
difficulties and severe characterological problems. After insight-oriented
treatment. a substantial number of patients were found to have changed for the
worse. The data confirmed that the "screened-off aspects of one's
self" are hidden for a reason; for some types of people the conflicts that
necessitated the screening off should remain hidden.
People diagnosed with obvious schizophreniform, bipolar and
other psychotics disorders appeared to have been generally excluded from est,
Actualizations (whose head man had been one of the first est trainers), PSI and
Lifespring. I cannot say that for sure about the other two. But, insofar as I
could tell, the rooms were full of people with various, pretty evident traits
of Diagnostic & Statistical Manual IV, Axis II "personality
disorders," including schizoid, paranoid, narcissistic,
histrionic, anti-social, passive-aggressive, obsessive-compulsive, masochistic, dependent and depressive disorders. I do not recall seeing (or otherwise sensing) any sadistic, schizotypal or grossly nihilistic traits.
Psychology, Applebaum argued, has to recognize the factors,
which impinge upon whether, when, how much, and what kind of insight a
particular person in particular circumstances should be helped to achieve. We
need to know . . . the patient's capacities in order to design the best amount,
kind, and timing of insight. (1976, pp. 205-206)
Most of the better psychotherapists and addiction counselors I have known over the past 30+ years assert that, "One size does NOT fit all," and that "One needs to meet the patient where the patient is."
The data demonstrated Applebaum's contention that differential
diagnosis and a differential treatment plan is crucial in effective
psychotherapy. This conclusion challenges the universal and absolutist claims
of insight oriented mass marathon groups. Applebaum warned that
· "Until we give up the pipe dream of insight as a
universal good or a universal bad, we and our patients will, at times, be
injured by its dangerous edge." (1976. p. 206)
Just
as Applebaum criticized those who considered the indiscriminate use of insight
a universal therapeutic panaceas so too did Hampden-Turner (1976) attack those
who treat human growth like a consumer product, indiscriminately applying certain
techniques to every customer who appears with a blank check. He vigorously
disagreed with the ethics of
· "The pop supermarket, the idea that you can purchase a
"peak" here and a "high" there, and go psycho-shopping for
prepackaged experiences…in fact human growth is not like a product at all, and
we vitiate it utterly by pandering to the consumer ethos." (1976, p. 3)
While "psycho-shopping" or "peak-seeking" was not a pop-psych pandemic in the early '70s, it certainly began to look that way in the '80s. Many of the recruits by then talked about the various LGATs they'd "done" pretty much like the drugs they'd done.
His critique focused particularly upon the highly structured,
authoritarian, insight-oriented marathon workshops. Some aspects of humanistic psychology, he argued, seem
· "…to have almost forgotten that our most precious human
values are achieved by indirection as opposed to the means ends rationality of
industrial production. . . I seriously question any high that has been
programmed in advance." (1976, pp. 1-2)
The rise of the MM/LGATs coincided with (or, at least shortly followed) the rise of the "human potential movement." And things fairly quickly got screwy there, as we all know.
He voiced his disagreement with psychological ideologies that
discount or deny the significance of the sociohistorical and economic realities
of the situation in which the client lives. These ideologies instead argue for
the grandiose delusion of the ultimate limitlessness of the individual. To the
organizations that teach this ideology he posed a provoking question:
· "If we are not aware of what the economy does to us,
are we self aware at all? Any genuine search for truth must remind us of the
things we cannot change." (1576, P. 3)
In this way Hampden-Turner raised an issue that Sampson (1981)
expanded upon. Sampson criticized cognitive psychology (the single most
prominent aspect of the ideology of many mass marathon organizations) for its
"subjectivist reduction." By this he meant the regressive tendency to
discount the nonsubjective world by considering it to be either a hallucination
or subject to the total control of the individual. According to some mass
marathon organizations, human fetuses choose their parents, female victims
choose to be raped, and Vietnamese children chose to be bombed. The regressive
aspects of this ideology seem to be readily apparent.
Sampson demonstrated how a regressive psychological doctrine
can impact on political activity. He argued that an ideology both accurately
expresses the "zeitgeist" of the era and may also inaccurately
distort the facts in order to serve the ruling elite. He explained how cognitivism
as an ideology serves the status quo of power and privilege in American society
by teaching individuals to reinterpret their internal response to a painful
experience rather than to work at rearranging the external situation so that it
could better facilitate personal and communal well being.
The sadly dichotomistic battle between the mostly pop psych proponents of internalizing vs. externalizing raged on for a good 30 or 40 years. It was always a case of going "black & white" and "all good vs. all bad" when it would better have been framed as "too much of a good thing may not be" in more Taoist and dialectical fashion. But the legions who claimed to understand Lao Tsu and even Marsha Linehan really didn't. (The embers are still hot, even in the era of Asian-influenced mind-body psychotherapies like Linehan's DBT, the Blocks' MBBT, Levine's SEPt, and Ogden's SP4T. Sigh.)
Humanistic psychology owes much to Lewin's "laboratory
movement, " which originally developed the encounter-group format at the
Bethel Institute. Mass marathon psychotherapy organizations claim that they are
within the legitimate tradition of this movement. They claim that their
training techniques, which include severe milieu control and a rigid ideology,
are taken directly from the encounter movement of years past.
By the late '60s, most of the cognoscenti in the Bay Area and El Lay's West Side knew the place to go to get laid by the world-class faces and bods was the weekend encounter group. I did several of Psychology of Self-Esteem author Nathaniel Branden's (which were not like the MM/LGAT's in terms of intentionally setting up the participants to work their way up any cultic pyramids). They were "artists & models" balls.
And yet
Gottschalk and Pattison's (1969) study of the history of T-groups and the
laboratory movement appears to refute that claim. They found that the
laboratory movement was originally an attempt to encourage democracy within
community action groups. It was composed of three types of groups: T-groups,
task-oriented groups dedicated to teaching about group process, and
intervention labs whose goals were action-oriented community improvement
programs. The authors found that the original unified effort diverged into an
increasing number of activities, each with different philosophical foundations
and agendas. The shift in the 1950s to "individual growth. . . . self
knowledge, to actualization and maturation" (1969, p. 4) was a clear
deviation from the founding philosophy.
They reported that T-group participants sometimes complained
of the hidden agendas, group norms, and covert values of charismatic group
leaders and their loyal followers.
"Hidden agendas" in human potential cults? Heavens! (The newcomer to all this may ask, "Well, what hidden agendas?" In my experience, all of the MM/LGATs were in fact recruiting and conditioning devices meant to indoctrinate, instruct, socialize, habituate and normalize as many of the participants as possible to the cult's beliefs and values so as to sell them on further involvement including unpaid service as volunteer recruiters, office workers and training personnel, as well as purchasers of downline trainings.)
They found that the trainer and various group members are
calling upon them to stop certain ways of behaving, talking, thinking, and
feeling, and that different ways of behaving are being prescribed. (p. 12)
Also the T-group was found to consciously evoke dramatic
reactions in the participants, which often involved an exaggeration of
impulsive traits and personality styles.
Gottschalk and Pattison isolated 13 liabilities of encounter
groups, some of which are similar to characteristics of most current mass
marathon psychotherapy training sessions:
They lack adequate participant-selection criteria.
They lack reliable norms, supervision, and adequate training
for leaders.
They lack clearly defined responsibility.
They sometimes foster pseudoauthenticity and pseudoreality.
Think "Tony Robbins and Joel Osteen on technological steroids."
They sometimes foster inappropriate patterns of relationships.
They sometimes ignore the necessity and utility of ego
defenses.
They sometimes teach the covert value of total exposure
instead of valuing personal differences.
They sometimes foster impulsive personality styles and
behavioral strategies.
They sometimes devalue critical thinking in favor of
"experiencing" without self-analysis or reflection.
Many MM/LGATs actually packaged and promoted their products as "critical thinking," which had a huge appeal to liberal arts majors at UCLA, Stanford and Berkeley at the time. While college students were not necessarily seen in large numbers among the basic training participants, graduates were.
They sometimes ignore stated goals, misrepresent their actual
techniques, and obfuscate their real agenda.
"Misrepresent(ing) their actual techniques" is fundamental in this world. "Enlightenment" means "enslavement" here. (Have a look at Cult Membership as a Behavioral Addiction ... 18 Examples of Baiting & Biting ... Chicken S--t vs. Chicken Salad ... The Effects of Double Binding ... Hypnotic Regression ... Groomed into Learned Helplessness ... The Abuse of Narrow Focus Meditation ... and The Typical Path. Decide for yourself.)
They sometimes focus too much on structural self-awareness
techniques and misplace the goal of democratic education; as a result
participants may learn more about themselves and less about group process.
They pay inadequate attention to decisions regarding time
limitations. This may lead to increased pressure on some participants to
unconsciously "fabricate" a cure.
I can't say I saw a lot of this in the lower level populations of the MM/LGATs, but I sure saw a lot of it in the medium-level hierarchy of the Church of Scientology... and levels five and up on the cultic pyramids of the MM/LGATs.
They fail to adequately consider the "psychonoxious"
or deleterious effects of group participation.... (1969, p. 13)
While this could be the case in the few situations where trainees were allowed to cross-talk during the trainings, it was farrrrrrrrrrrr more widespread phenomenon among the mid-level (fives to eights) on the pyramids for sure. Wholesale public humiliation was relentless at those levels, and used as a shaming device by the trainers in the trainings after the first two days or relative "playing nice." To be a staff member or training volunteer was to be a masochist in training.
As a result, participants and leaders may unconsciously
distort their feelings and responses when reporting to researchers about the
group or recruiting for future groups. This might result in a deceptive
"oversell" that could undermine informed consent and lead to
unrealistic regressive expectations in new recruits, the specific type of
problems that have been found to lead to psychological casualties (see Yalom
& Lieberman, 1972, below). Since these liabilities are so similar to the
techniques used in some mass marathon training's, they may also cause
psychological damage in that setting as well.
In a significant study with far-reaching consequences for the
study of mass marathon training's, Yalom and Lieberman (1972) observed in 209
undergraduate subjects the negative effects of participation in an encounter
group. Over the course of 10 weeks, 18 groups met for 30 hours; there were also
150 fifty control subjects who did not attend any group.
Each group was run by a leader who was chosen because he was
an excellent representative of one of 10 ideological schools of encounter
(T-groups, Gestalt, Rogerian-marathon, psychodrama, psychoanalytic,
Transactional Analysis, sensory awareness, Synanon, personal growth,
black-white encounter, and leaderless). Each was given complete freedom.
Yalom and Lieberman's primary interest was in assessing the
types and causes of psychiatric "casualties." The operational
definition of a casualty was "an enduring, significant, negative outcome
which . . . was caused by…participation in the group" (1972, p. 223).
There is little doubt that the careful, conservative manner in which the study
was conducted tended to minimize negative results and reduce the risk to
subjects (1972, p. 228). The authors developed a system for identifying
subjects who were harmed. Their definition of this subsample and their means of
locating it were characteristically conservative. Subjects were included in the
casualty subsample only when they had experienced "enduring" negative
change and
· "…as a direct result of . . . [their] experience in the
encounter group became more psychologically distressed and/or employed more
maladaptive mechanisms of defense." (1972. p. 228)
Also, the experience must have been proven to be the
responsible element in the psychological decompensation. For example, one
subject committed suicide during the study and was not counted as a casualty
because the suicide could have been caused from past encounter group
experiences.
In a startling finding, Yalom and Lieberman reported that 9.4%
of the subjects met their stringent criteria and were therefore identified as
casualties. The authors viewed this as a serious challenge to the entire
movement.
The authors also determined that it was neither the
psychological traits of the subjects (i.e., predispositional factors) nor the
ideology of the leaders (i.e., doctrinal factors) that determined the casualty
rate. Instead, surprisingly, it was the style of leadership that was primary.
Leaders who were aggressive, stimulating, intrusive, confrontive, challenging,
personally revealing, and authoritarian were the leaders who caused the
casualties.
Specifically these leaders often unilaterally structured the
group's events. Their focus was on the individual rather than group process.
They provided a comprehensive intellectual framework with which to understand
one's self and one's world. They exercised firm control and were "ready,
willing and able" to take over for participants and guide them to
"enlightenment" (1972, p. 236). They were people who were charismatic
leaders: they had a universal message to deliver, a foolproof technique to use,
and a cause to recruit for. They were uninhibited in their attempts to convert
all the participants in their group. These characteristics are clearly
duplicated by many mass marathon trainers. The findings corroborated Gottschalk
and Pattison's 1969 conclusions and again call into question many tactics used
by mass marathon organizations.
Of the categories that caused casualties,
"rejection" was the most damaging. "Failure to achieve
unrealistic goals" was the second most dangerous category. Each of these
subjects reported being pressed for a breakthrough without being able to
deliver. "Leader attack"-"group attack" tied for third. The
fact that participants were restrained from leaving, that they had "no
place to hide," was thought to be a crucial element. [As is the case in any form of conditioning to "learned helplessness."] "Group pressure
to experience and express feelings" also caused casualties. When subjects
couldn't comply, they felt a "sense of hollowness" which led to a
"deficient or empty self-image" (1972, p. 243).
Interestingly, many subjects who demographically resembled the
casualty subsample didn't have negative experiences. Yalom and Lieberman found
that they had more realistic expectations for the experience, they were not
lonely or depressed, they remained uninvolved (i.e. "…they did not enter
into a public confessional and therefore maintained their objectivity and their
'observing ego'"), they dropped out of the group, they depended on a
positive self-concept when they were negatively criticized by the group, or
they used an outside reference group to bolster their own beliefs when in
conflict with a group norm.
The authors suggested that a questionnaire that detects
unrealistic expectations would be a helpful counterindicator when attempting to
predict which potential participants would be at risk. In summary, Yalom and
Lieberman stressed that casualties were caused by the style and techniques of
the leader, and by recruitment and selection practices.
The groups were determined to be dangerous when:
Leaders had rigid, unbending beliefs about what participants
should experience and believe, how they should behave in the group, and when
they should change.
Leaders had no sense of differential diagnosis and assessment
skills, valued cathartic emotional breakthroughs as the ultimate therapeutic
experience, and sadistically pressed to create or force a breakthrough in every
participant.
Leaders had an evangelical system of belief that was the one
single pathway to salvation.
Leaders were true believers and sealed their doctrine off from
discomforting data or disquieting results and tended to discount a poor result
by, "blaming the victim."
Yalom and Lieberman concluded by again emphasizing the crucial
importance of informed consent. "Our best means of prevention," they
maintained, remains the type of group the subject enters, and our best means
for prevention is self-selection. If responsible public education can teach
prospective encounter group members about what they can expect in terms of process,
risks, and profits from a certain type of group, then and only then can they
make an informed decision about membership. (p. 253)
It is instructive to note that many mass marathon
organizations are conducting their training in the exact manner found by Yalom
and Lieberman to cause the greatest number of psychiatric casualties.
In my own experience, as well as encounters with other MM/LGAT participants after the fact, there's an even worse, long-term upshot: Many people emerge from their long-term experiences moving up the side of the cultic pyramid to the sixth through eighth levels where the abuse is the worst stuck with powerful associations between the actual (mostly existential) facts delivered during the sessions and the abuse. And that association often precludes ever using those facts for the psychotherapeutic purpose for which those facts were originally intended: Escape from the common cultural concensus trance to which most people have been conditioned, etc., by the messages and modeling of a much larger "commercial cult." Several exitors I know who came to understand and work their way out of the CT, have told me that that was what they were hoping to find when they bought their ticket for the MM/LGAT to begin with.
Note: For a detailed, day-by-day overview of the training process
see a "Description
of the Behavioral Structure of the Training."
Commentator's References & Resources
The mass marathon trainings (aka "large group awareness trainings" or LGATs) discussed in Cushman's work are pretty clearly of the sort popular in the 1970s and '80s, including the following (with all of which I am personally familiar):
In my own experience, as well as encounters with other MM/LGAT participants after the fact, there's an even worse, long-term upshot: Many people emerge from their long-term experiences moving up the side of the cultic pyramid to the sixth through eighth levels where the abuse is the worst stuck with powerful associations between the actual (mostly existential) facts delivered during the sessions and the abuse. And that association often precludes ever using those facts for the psychotherapeutic purpose for which those facts were originally intended: Escape from the common cultural concensus trance to which most people have been conditioned, etc., by the messages and modeling of a much larger "commercial cult." Several exitors I know who came to understand and work their way out of the CT, have told me that that was what they were hoping to find when they bought their ticket for the MM/LGAT to begin with.